Fahnestock and Secor’s article on Stasis patterns aims to
inform readers on the construction of arguments. They claim that “the stases
tell the writer where to think, not “what to think.” As writers it is useful to
study how to effectively construct an argument. Much like Grant- Davie’s idea
of exigence, Fahnestock and Secor synthesize stasis into a formulaic pattern
that assesses the purpose and timeliness of arguments. Questions of causes are
similar to exigence in that they define “what the discourse is a bout, why it
is needed, and what it should accomplish (Grant Davie 266). Last class my group
examined a sci/tech article on binge drinking. The stasis was clear in that
unexpected facts were presented to break down barriers of assumption. We have
all heard about binge drinking problems in universities and amongst the youth.
The article took a unique stance and informed the reader via age groups and sex
(male/female). Surprising facts insinuated that the article was not intended for
current binge drinkers, but for individuals in positions that have the
capability of making changes. The stasis patterns in scientific discourse and
literary criticism both take predisposed assumptions on a topic and try to
change that discipline in a new direction. I find it interesting that science
arguments are conducted in lower stases. This is probably due to the fact that
science results are less philosophical and more concrete.
Killingsworth and Palmer explore the concepts of news and
common interest. “To be considered news it must tell readers something they
don’t already know, something they haven’t already heard or become accustomed
to.” Common interest revolves more around “taking an unusual approach to an old
problem” (Killingsworth and Palmer 134). An interesting point arises in the
application of common interest to scientific rhetoric. Common interest
encompasses social aspects in that it takes a socially rooted problem, such as
binge drinking, and uses statistical data to analyze the situation. It is
important to build discourse in our highly mediated society because it is naïve
to only look at scientific studies through the eyes of “numbers”. At this point
in time scientists need to be thinking outside of the box to reach new
conclusions. For example, the genetic predisposition of alcohol abuse has been
circling the media for a while now. A scientific study is more likely to be
effective and useful if it attaches socially constructed paradigms.
Killingsworth and Palmer’s discussion of community formation clarify Fahnestock
and Secor’s stasis levels in that a clear and leveled audience helps deliver
messages to a broader spectrum. The reader of a scientific article does not
have to be an expert on the topic to understand what the discourse is. This is
achieved through the stasis patterns that help us (the reader) organize and
analyze statements. The shift from “news” to “public” involvement is depicted
in Time magazines coverage on
environmentalism. I believe the best depiction of this shift is their 1988
issue. Placing the article The Shift
toward Environmental Awareness in
the “Nation” section as opposed to the “Environment” section instantly changes
the predisposed opinions of the reader. Public awareness arises by categorizing
the issue on a national level as opposed to an environmental one. The discourse
hints that the nation needs to make a change, not the environment.
No comments:
Post a Comment